Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.
Non-Experimental Research
32 Observational Research
Learning objectives.
- List the various types of observational research methods and distinguish between each.
- Describe the strengths and weakness of each observational research method.
What Is Observational Research?
The term observational research is used to refer to several different types of non-experimental studies in which behavior is systematically observed and recorded. The goal of observational research is to describe a variable or set of variables. More generally, the goal is to obtain a snapshot of specific characteristics of an individual, group, or setting. As described previously, observational research is non-experimental because nothing is manipulated or controlled, and as such we cannot arrive at causal conclusions using this approach. The data that are collected in observational research studies are often qualitative in nature but they may also be quantitative or both (mixed-methods). There are several different types of observational methods that will be described below.
Naturalistic Observation
Naturalistic observation is an observational method that involves observing people’s behavior in the environment in which it typically occurs. Thus naturalistic observation is a type of field research (as opposed to a type of laboratory research). Jane Goodall’s famous research on chimpanzees is a classic example of naturalistic observation. Dr. Goodall spent three decades observing chimpanzees in their natural environment in East Africa. She examined such things as chimpanzee’s social structure, mating patterns, gender roles, family structure, and care of offspring by observing them in the wild. However, naturalistic observation could more simply involve observing shoppers in a grocery store, children on a school playground, or psychiatric inpatients in their wards. Researchers engaged in naturalistic observation usually make their observations as unobtrusively as possible so that participants are not aware that they are being studied. Such an approach is called disguised naturalistic observation . Ethically, this method is considered to be acceptable if the participants remain anonymous and the behavior occurs in a public setting where people would not normally have an expectation of privacy. Grocery shoppers putting items into their shopping carts, for example, are engaged in public behavior that is easily observable by store employees and other shoppers. For this reason, most researchers would consider it ethically acceptable to observe them for a study. On the other hand, one of the arguments against the ethicality of the naturalistic observation of “bathroom behavior” discussed earlier in the book is that people have a reasonable expectation of privacy even in a public restroom and that this expectation was violated.
In cases where it is not ethical or practical to conduct disguised naturalistic observation, researchers can conduct undisguised naturalistic observation where the participants are made aware of the researcher presence and monitoring of their behavior. However, one concern with undisguised naturalistic observation is reactivity. Reactivity refers to when a measure changes participants’ behavior. In the case of undisguised naturalistic observation, the concern with reactivity is that when people know they are being observed and studied, they may act differently than they normally would. This type of reactivity is known as the Hawthorne effect . For instance, you may act much differently in a bar if you know that someone is observing you and recording your behaviors and this would invalidate the study. So disguised observation is less reactive and therefore can have higher validity because people are not aware that their behaviors are being observed and recorded. However, we now know that people often become used to being observed and with time they begin to behave naturally in the researcher’s presence. In other words, over time people habituate to being observed. Think about reality shows like Big Brother or Survivor where people are constantly being observed and recorded. While they may be on their best behavior at first, in a fairly short amount of time they are flirting, having sex, wearing next to nothing, screaming at each other, and occasionally behaving in ways that are embarrassing.
Participant Observation
Another approach to data collection in observational research is participant observation. In participant observation , researchers become active participants in the group or situation they are studying. Participant observation is very similar to naturalistic observation in that it involves observing people’s behavior in the environment in which it typically occurs. As with naturalistic observation, the data that are collected can include interviews (usually unstructured), notes based on their observations and interactions, documents, photographs, and other artifacts. The only difference between naturalistic observation and participant observation is that researchers engaged in participant observation become active members of the group or situations they are studying. The basic rationale for participant observation is that there may be important information that is only accessible to, or can be interpreted only by, someone who is an active participant in the group or situation. Like naturalistic observation, participant observation can be either disguised or undisguised. In disguised participant observation , the researchers pretend to be members of the social group they are observing and conceal their true identity as researchers.
In a famous example of disguised participant observation, Leon Festinger and his colleagues infiltrated a doomsday cult known as the Seekers, whose members believed that the apocalypse would occur on December 21, 1954. Interested in studying how members of the group would cope psychologically when the prophecy inevitably failed, they carefully recorded the events and reactions of the cult members in the days before and after the supposed end of the world. Unsurprisingly, the cult members did not give up their belief but instead convinced themselves that it was their faith and efforts that saved the world from destruction. Festinger and his colleagues later published a book about this experience, which they used to illustrate the theory of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, Riecken, & Schachter, 1956) [1] .
In contrast with undisguised participant observation , the researchers become a part of the group they are studying and they disclose their true identity as researchers to the group under investigation. Once again there are important ethical issues to consider with disguised participant observation. First no informed consent can be obtained and second deception is being used. The researcher is deceiving the participants by intentionally withholding information about their motivations for being a part of the social group they are studying. But sometimes disguised participation is the only way to access a protective group (like a cult). Further, disguised participant observation is less prone to reactivity than undisguised participant observation.
Rosenhan’s study (1973) [2] of the experience of people in a psychiatric ward would be considered disguised participant observation because Rosenhan and his pseudopatients were admitted into psychiatric hospitals on the pretense of being patients so that they could observe the way that psychiatric patients are treated by staff. The staff and other patients were unaware of their true identities as researchers.
Another example of participant observation comes from a study by sociologist Amy Wilkins on a university-based religious organization that emphasized how happy its members were (Wilkins, 2008) [3] . Wilkins spent 12 months attending and participating in the group’s meetings and social events, and she interviewed several group members. In her study, Wilkins identified several ways in which the group “enforced” happiness—for example, by continually talking about happiness, discouraging the expression of negative emotions, and using happiness as a way to distinguish themselves from other groups.
One of the primary benefits of participant observation is that the researchers are in a much better position to understand the viewpoint and experiences of the people they are studying when they are a part of the social group. The primary limitation with this approach is that the mere presence of the observer could affect the behavior of the people being observed. While this is also a concern with naturalistic observation, additional concerns arise when researchers become active members of the social group they are studying because that they may change the social dynamics and/or influence the behavior of the people they are studying. Similarly, if the researcher acts as a participant observer there can be concerns with biases resulting from developing relationships with the participants. Concretely, the researcher may become less objective resulting in more experimenter bias.
Structured Observation
Another observational method is structured observation . Here the investigator makes careful observations of one or more specific behaviors in a particular setting that is more structured than the settings used in naturalistic or participant observation. Often the setting in which the observations are made is not the natural setting. Instead, the researcher may observe people in the laboratory environment. Alternatively, the researcher may observe people in a natural setting (like a classroom setting) that they have structured some way, for instance by introducing some specific task participants are to engage in or by introducing a specific social situation or manipulation.
Structured observation is very similar to naturalistic observation and participant observation in that in all three cases researchers are observing naturally occurring behavior; however, the emphasis in structured observation is on gathering quantitative rather than qualitative data. Researchers using this approach are interested in a limited set of behaviors. This allows them to quantify the behaviors they are observing. In other words, structured observation is less global than naturalistic or participant observation because the researcher engaged in structured observations is interested in a small number of specific behaviors. Therefore, rather than recording everything that happens, the researcher only focuses on very specific behaviors of interest.
Researchers Robert Levine and Ara Norenzayan used structured observation to study differences in the “pace of life” across countries (Levine & Norenzayan, 1999) [4] . One of their measures involved observing pedestrians in a large city to see how long it took them to walk 60 feet. They found that people in some countries walked reliably faster than people in other countries. For example, people in Canada and Sweden covered 60 feet in just under 13 seconds on average, while people in Brazil and Romania took close to 17 seconds. When structured observation takes place in the complex and even chaotic “real world,” the questions of when, where, and under what conditions the observations will be made, and who exactly will be observed are important to consider. Levine and Norenzayan described their sampling process as follows:
“Male and female walking speed over a distance of 60 feet was measured in at least two locations in main downtown areas in each city. Measurements were taken during main business hours on clear summer days. All locations were flat, unobstructed, had broad sidewalks, and were sufficiently uncrowded to allow pedestrians to move at potentially maximum speeds. To control for the effects of socializing, only pedestrians walking alone were used. Children, individuals with obvious physical handicaps, and window-shoppers were not timed. Thirty-five men and 35 women were timed in most cities.” (p. 186).
Precise specification of the sampling process in this way makes data collection manageable for the observers, and it also provides some control over important extraneous variables. For example, by making their observations on clear summer days in all countries, Levine and Norenzayan controlled for effects of the weather on people’s walking speeds. In Levine and Norenzayan’s study, measurement was relatively straightforward. They simply measured out a 60-foot distance along a city sidewalk and then used a stopwatch to time participants as they walked over that distance.
As another example, researchers Robert Kraut and Robert Johnston wanted to study bowlers’ reactions to their shots, both when they were facing the pins and then when they turned toward their companions (Kraut & Johnston, 1979) [5] . But what “reactions” should they observe? Based on previous research and their own pilot testing, Kraut and Johnston created a list of reactions that included “closed smile,” “open smile,” “laugh,” “neutral face,” “look down,” “look away,” and “face cover” (covering one’s face with one’s hands). The observers committed this list to memory and then practiced by coding the reactions of bowlers who had been videotaped. During the actual study, the observers spoke into an audio recorder, describing the reactions they observed. Among the most interesting results of this study was that bowlers rarely smiled while they still faced the pins. They were much more likely to smile after they turned toward their companions, suggesting that smiling is not purely an expression of happiness but also a form of social communication.
In yet another example (this one in a laboratory environment), Dov Cohen and his colleagues had observers rate the emotional reactions of participants who had just been deliberately bumped and insulted by a confederate after they dropped off a completed questionnaire at the end of a hallway. The confederate was posing as someone who worked in the same building and who was frustrated by having to close a file drawer twice in order to permit the participants to walk past them (first to drop off the questionnaire at the end of the hallway and once again on their way back to the room where they believed the study they signed up for was taking place). The two observers were positioned at different ends of the hallway so that they could read the participants’ body language and hear anything they might say. Interestingly, the researchers hypothesized that participants from the southern United States, which is one of several places in the world that has a “culture of honor,” would react with more aggression than participants from the northern United States, a prediction that was in fact supported by the observational data (Cohen, Nisbett, Bowdle, & Schwarz, 1996) [6] .
When the observations require a judgment on the part of the observers—as in the studies by Kraut and Johnston and Cohen and his colleagues—a process referred to as coding is typically required . Coding generally requires clearly defining a set of target behaviors. The observers then categorize participants individually in terms of which behavior they have engaged in and the number of times they engaged in each behavior. The observers might even record the duration of each behavior. The target behaviors must be defined in such a way that guides different observers to code them in the same way. This difficulty with coding illustrates the issue of interrater reliability, as mentioned in Chapter 4. Researchers are expected to demonstrate the interrater reliability of their coding procedure by having multiple raters code the same behaviors independently and then showing that the different observers are in close agreement. Kraut and Johnston, for example, video recorded a subset of their participants’ reactions and had two observers independently code them. The two observers showed that they agreed on the reactions that were exhibited 97% of the time, indicating good interrater reliability.
One of the primary benefits of structured observation is that it is far more efficient than naturalistic and participant observation. Since the researchers are focused on specific behaviors this reduces time and expense. Also, often times the environment is structured to encourage the behaviors of interest which again means that researchers do not have to invest as much time in waiting for the behaviors of interest to naturally occur. Finally, researchers using this approach can clearly exert greater control over the environment. However, when researchers exert more control over the environment it may make the environment less natural which decreases external validity. It is less clear for instance whether structured observations made in a laboratory environment will generalize to a real world environment. Furthermore, since researchers engaged in structured observation are often not disguised there may be more concerns with reactivity.
Case Studies
A case study is an in-depth examination of an individual. Sometimes case studies are also completed on social units (e.g., a cult) and events (e.g., a natural disaster). Most commonly in psychology, however, case studies provide a detailed description and analysis of an individual. Often the individual has a rare or unusual condition or disorder or has damage to a specific region of the brain.
Like many observational research methods, case studies tend to be more qualitative in nature. Case study methods involve an in-depth, and often a longitudinal examination of an individual. Depending on the focus of the case study, individuals may or may not be observed in their natural setting. If the natural setting is not what is of interest, then the individual may be brought into a therapist’s office or a researcher’s lab for study. Also, the bulk of the case study report will focus on in-depth descriptions of the person rather than on statistical analyses. With that said some quantitative data may also be included in the write-up of a case study. For instance, an individual’s depression score may be compared to normative scores or their score before and after treatment may be compared. As with other qualitative methods, a variety of different methods and tools can be used to collect information on the case. For instance, interviews, naturalistic observation, structured observation, psychological testing (e.g., IQ test), and/or physiological measurements (e.g., brain scans) may be used to collect information on the individual.
HM is one of the most notorious case studies in psychology. HM suffered from intractable and very severe epilepsy. A surgeon localized HM’s epilepsy to his medial temporal lobe and in 1953 he removed large sections of his hippocampus in an attempt to stop the seizures. The treatment was a success, in that it resolved his epilepsy and his IQ and personality were unaffected. However, the doctors soon realized that HM exhibited a strange form of amnesia, called anterograde amnesia. HM was able to carry out a conversation and he could remember short strings of letters, digits, and words. Basically, his short term memory was preserved. However, HM could not commit new events to memory. He lost the ability to transfer information from his short-term memory to his long term memory, something memory researchers call consolidation. So while he could carry on a conversation with someone, he would completely forget the conversation after it ended. This was an extremely important case study for memory researchers because it suggested that there’s a dissociation between short-term memory and long-term memory, it suggested that these were two different abilities sub-served by different areas of the brain. It also suggested that the temporal lobes are particularly important for consolidating new information (i.e., for transferring information from short-term memory to long-term memory).
The history of psychology is filled with influential cases studies, such as Sigmund Freud’s description of “Anna O.” (see Note 6.1 “The Case of “Anna O.””) and John Watson and Rosalie Rayner’s description of Little Albert (Watson & Rayner, 1920) [7] , who allegedly learned to fear a white rat—along with other furry objects—when the researchers repeatedly made a loud noise every time the rat approached him.
The Case of “Anna O.”
Sigmund Freud used the case of a young woman he called “Anna O.” to illustrate many principles of his theory of psychoanalysis (Freud, 1961) [8] . (Her real name was Bertha Pappenheim, and she was an early feminist who went on to make important contributions to the field of social work.) Anna had come to Freud’s colleague Josef Breuer around 1880 with a variety of odd physical and psychological symptoms. One of them was that for several weeks she was unable to drink any fluids. According to Freud,
She would take up the glass of water that she longed for, but as soon as it touched her lips she would push it away like someone suffering from hydrophobia.…She lived only on fruit, such as melons, etc., so as to lessen her tormenting thirst. (p. 9)
But according to Freud, a breakthrough came one day while Anna was under hypnosis.
[S]he grumbled about her English “lady-companion,” whom she did not care for, and went on to describe, with every sign of disgust, how she had once gone into this lady’s room and how her little dog—horrid creature!—had drunk out of a glass there. The patient had said nothing, as she had wanted to be polite. After giving further energetic expression to the anger she had held back, she asked for something to drink, drank a large quantity of water without any difficulty, and awoke from her hypnosis with the glass at her lips; and thereupon the disturbance vanished, never to return. (p.9)
Freud’s interpretation was that Anna had repressed the memory of this incident along with the emotion that it triggered and that this was what had caused her inability to drink. Furthermore, he believed that her recollection of the incident, along with her expression of the emotion she had repressed, caused the symptom to go away.
As an illustration of Freud’s theory, the case study of Anna O. is quite effective. As evidence for the theory, however, it is essentially worthless. The description provides no way of knowing whether Anna had really repressed the memory of the dog drinking from the glass, whether this repression had caused her inability to drink, or whether recalling this “trauma” relieved the symptom. It is also unclear from this case study how typical or atypical Anna’s experience was.
Case studies are useful because they provide a level of detailed analysis not found in many other research methods and greater insights may be gained from this more detailed analysis. As a result of the case study, the researcher may gain a sharpened understanding of what might become important to look at more extensively in future more controlled research. Case studies are also often the only way to study rare conditions because it may be impossible to find a large enough sample of individuals with the condition to use quantitative methods. Although at first glance a case study of a rare individual might seem to tell us little about ourselves, they often do provide insights into normal behavior. The case of HM provided important insights into the role of the hippocampus in memory consolidation.
However, it is important to note that while case studies can provide insights into certain areas and variables to study, and can be useful in helping develop theories, they should never be used as evidence for theories. In other words, case studies can be used as inspiration to formulate theories and hypotheses, but those hypotheses and theories then need to be formally tested using more rigorous quantitative methods. The reason case studies shouldn’t be used to provide support for theories is that they suffer from problems with both internal and external validity. Case studies lack the proper controls that true experiments contain. As such, they suffer from problems with internal validity, so they cannot be used to determine causation. For instance, during HM’s surgery, the surgeon may have accidentally lesioned another area of HM’s brain (a possibility suggested by the dissection of HM’s brain following his death) and that lesion may have contributed to his inability to consolidate new information. The fact is, with case studies we cannot rule out these sorts of alternative explanations. So, as with all observational methods, case studies do not permit determination of causation. In addition, because case studies are often of a single individual, and typically an abnormal individual, researchers cannot generalize their conclusions to other individuals. Recall that with most research designs there is a trade-off between internal and external validity. With case studies, however, there are problems with both internal validity and external validity. So there are limits both to the ability to determine causation and to generalize the results. A final limitation of case studies is that ample opportunity exists for the theoretical biases of the researcher to color or bias the case description. Indeed, there have been accusations that the woman who studied HM destroyed a lot of her data that were not published and she has been called into question for destroying contradictory data that didn’t support her theory about how memories are consolidated. There is a fascinating New York Times article that describes some of the controversies that ensued after HM’s death and analysis of his brain that can be found at: https://www.nytimes.com/2016/08/07/magazine/the-brain-that-couldnt-remember.html?_r=0
Archival Research
Another approach that is often considered observational research involves analyzing archival data that have already been collected for some other purpose. An example is a study by Brett Pelham and his colleagues on “implicit egotism”—the tendency for people to prefer people, places, and things that are similar to themselves (Pelham, Carvallo, & Jones, 2005) [9] . In one study, they examined Social Security records to show that women with the names Virginia, Georgia, Louise, and Florence were especially likely to have moved to the states of Virginia, Georgia, Louisiana, and Florida, respectively.
As with naturalistic observation, measurement can be more or less straightforward when working with archival data. For example, counting the number of people named Virginia who live in various states based on Social Security records is relatively straightforward. But consider a study by Christopher Peterson and his colleagues on the relationship between optimism and health using data that had been collected many years before for a study on adult development (Peterson, Seligman, & Vaillant, 1988) [10] . In the 1940s, healthy male college students had completed an open-ended questionnaire about difficult wartime experiences. In the late 1980s, Peterson and his colleagues reviewed the men’s questionnaire responses to obtain a measure of explanatory style—their habitual ways of explaining bad events that happen to them. More pessimistic people tend to blame themselves and expect long-term negative consequences that affect many aspects of their lives, while more optimistic people tend to blame outside forces and expect limited negative consequences. To obtain a measure of explanatory style for each participant, the researchers used a procedure in which all negative events mentioned in the questionnaire responses, and any causal explanations for them were identified and written on index cards. These were given to a separate group of raters who rated each explanation in terms of three separate dimensions of optimism-pessimism. These ratings were then averaged to produce an explanatory style score for each participant. The researchers then assessed the statistical relationship between the men’s explanatory style as undergraduate students and archival measures of their health at approximately 60 years of age. The primary result was that the more optimistic the men were as undergraduate students, the healthier they were as older men. Pearson’s r was +.25.
This method is an example of content analysis —a family of systematic approaches to measurement using complex archival data. Just as structured observation requires specifying the behaviors of interest and then noting them as they occur, content analysis requires specifying keywords, phrases, or ideas and then finding all occurrences of them in the data. These occurrences can then be counted, timed (e.g., the amount of time devoted to entertainment topics on the nightly news show), or analyzed in a variety of other ways.
Media Attributions
- What happens when you remove the hippocampus? – Sam Kean by TED-Ed licensed under a standard YouTube License
- Pappenheim 1882 by unknown is in the Public Domain .
- Festinger, L., Riecken, H., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails: A social and psychological study of a modern group that predicted the destruction of the world. University of Minnesota Press. ↵
- Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). On being sane in insane places. Science, 179 , 250–258. ↵
- Wilkins, A. (2008). “Happier than Non-Christians”: Collective emotions and symbolic boundaries among evangelical Christians. Social Psychology Quarterly, 71 , 281–301. ↵
- Levine, R. V., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30 , 178–205. ↵
- Kraut, R. E., & Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social and emotional messages of smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37 , 1539–1553. ↵
- Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An "experimental ethnography." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 (5), 945-960. ↵
- Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3 , 1–14. ↵
- Freud, S. (1961). Five lectures on psycho-analysis . New York, NY: Norton. ↵
- Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., & Jones, J. T. (2005). Implicit egotism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14 , 106–110. ↵
- Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., & Vaillant, G. E. (1988). Pessimistic explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55 , 23–27. ↵
Research that is non-experimental because it focuses on recording systemic observations of behavior in a natural or laboratory setting without manipulating anything.
An observational method that involves observing people’s behavior in the environment in which it typically occurs.
When researchers engage in naturalistic observation by making their observations as unobtrusively as possible so that participants are not aware that they are being studied.
Where the participants are made aware of the researcher presence and monitoring of their behavior.
Refers to when a measure changes participants’ behavior.
In the case of undisguised naturalistic observation, it is a type of reactivity when people know they are being observed and studied, they may act differently than they normally would.
Researchers become active participants in the group or situation they are studying.
Researchers pretend to be members of the social group they are observing and conceal their true identity as researchers.
Researchers become a part of the group they are studying and they disclose their true identity as researchers to the group under investigation.
When a researcher makes careful observations of one or more specific behaviors in a particular setting that is more structured than the settings used in naturalistic or participant observation.
A part of structured observation whereby the observers use a clearly defined set of guidelines to "code" behaviors—assigning specific behaviors they are observing to a category—and count the number of times or the duration that the behavior occurs.
An in-depth examination of an individual.
A family of systematic approaches to measurement using qualitative methods to analyze complex archival data.
Research Methods in Psychology Copyright © 2019 by Rajiv S. Jhangiani, I-Chant A. Chiang, Carrie Cuttler, & Dana C. Leighton is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.
Share This Book
- General Categories
- Mental Health
- IQ and Intelligence
- Bipolar Disorder
Observation Method in Psychology: A Comprehensive Exploration of Research Techniques
From the unobtrusive researcher jotting down notes in a crowded café to the meticulous scientist analyzing hours of video footage, the observation method has long been a cornerstone of psychological research, offering an unparalleled window into the intricacies of human behavior. This powerful approach to understanding the human mind and its manifestations in real-world settings has captivated researchers for generations, providing rich insights that laboratory experiments alone often fail to capture.
Imagine, if you will, a world where our understanding of human nature was limited to self-reports and controlled experiments. We’d be missing out on the subtle nuances of social interactions, the spontaneous expressions of emotion, and the complex dance of human relationships that unfold in natural settings. This is where the observation method truly shines, allowing psychologists to peek behind the curtain of everyday life and uncover the hidden patterns that shape our behavior.
But what exactly do we mean when we talk about observation in psychology? At its core, observational research involves systematically watching and recording behavior in natural or controlled settings. It’s not just about casual people-watching (though that can be fun too!). Instead, it’s a rigorous scientific method that requires careful planning, meticulous documentation, and thoughtful analysis.
The importance of observational research in psychological studies cannot be overstated. It allows us to study behaviors that might be difficult or unethical to manipulate in a lab setting. For instance, how do children naturally develop social skills on a playground? How do couples navigate conflicts in their own homes? These are questions that naturalistic observation in psychology is uniquely positioned to answer.
The history of observational methods in psychology is as fascinating as it is long. From Charles Darwin’s detailed observations of his own children’s emotional expressions to Jane Goodall’s groundbreaking studies of chimpanzees in the wild, observation has been a crucial tool in advancing our understanding of both human and animal behavior. These early pioneers laid the groundwork for what would become a robust and diverse set of observational techniques used in modern psychological research.
Types of Observation Methods in Psychology
Now, let’s dive into the various flavors of observational research. It’s not a one-size-fits-all approach – psychologists have developed a range of techniques to suit different research questions and contexts.
First up, we have naturalistic observation. This is the fly-on-the-wall approach, where researchers observe behavior in its natural habitat without interfering. Picture a psychologist discretely observing children’s play patterns in a public park or studying customer behavior in a bustling shopping mall. The beauty of this method lies in its authenticity – you’re seeing behavior as it naturally occurs, warts and all.
On the flip side, we have participant observation, where the researcher becomes part of the group they’re studying. This method is particularly popular in anthropology and sociology, but it has its place in psychology too. Imagine a researcher joining a support group for anxiety sufferers to gain firsthand insights into their experiences. It’s a delicate balance of participation and observation, requiring researchers to walk the tightrope between involvement and objectivity.
Laboratory observation in psychology offers a more controlled environment. Here, researchers can manipulate certain variables while still observing natural behavior. Think of a study where children are brought into a playroom with various toys, and researchers observe their play patterns and social interactions. It’s a bit like creating a mini-world where certain aspects can be controlled, but behavior can still unfold naturally.
Another important distinction is between structured and unstructured observation. Structured observation in psychology involves a predetermined set of categories or behaviors to look out for. It’s like having a checklist of specific actions or interactions you’re interested in. Unstructured observation, on the other hand, is more open-ended. Researchers might start with a general area of interest but allow their observations to guide them to unexpected discoveries.
Lastly, we have the distinction between covert and overt observation. Covert observation is when subjects don’t know they’re being observed – think hidden cameras or researchers posing as regular members of a group. Overt observation in psychology , by contrast, is when participants are aware they’re being studied. Both approaches have their pros and cons, and the choice often depends on the research question and ethical considerations.
Key Components of the Observational Method
Now that we’ve got a handle on the types of observation, let’s break down the key components that make up a solid observational study. It’s not just about watching and taking notes – there’s a whole science to doing it right!
First things first: selecting the research question and subjects. This is where the rubber meets the road in terms of what you want to study and who you want to study it in. Are you interested in how toddlers develop language skills? Or maybe you’re curious about how people behave in emergency situations? Your research question will guide everything that follows, so it’s crucial to get this step right.
Once you’ve nailed down your question, it’s time to choose the appropriate observation technique. Will naturalistic observation give you the insights you need, or would a more controlled setting be better? Perhaps a mix of methods would work best. It’s like choosing the right tool for the job – you wouldn’t use a hammer to paint a wall, right?
Developing observation protocols and tools is the next critical step. This is where you decide exactly what you’re going to look for and how you’re going to record it. Will you use a checklist of behaviors? A rating scale? Maybe you’ll need specialized software to code complex interactions. Whatever you choose, it needs to be systematic and reliable.
Speaking of reliability, training observers is crucial for consistency. If you have multiple people observing and recording data, you need to make sure they’re all on the same page. This might involve practice sessions, discussions about how to interpret certain behaviors, and regular check-ins to ensure everyone’s still seeing eye to eye.
Finally, there’s the all-important task of recording and documenting observations. In the old days, this might have involved furious scribbling in notebooks. Today, we have a wealth of tools at our disposal, from high-tech video recording systems to sophisticated data analysis software. The key is to capture as much relevant detail as possible without getting bogged down in unnecessary information.
Advantages of the Observation Method in Psychology
Alright, let’s talk about why observation is such a powerhouse in the psychologist’s toolkit. There are some serious perks to this method that make it invaluable in certain research contexts.
First and foremost, observation allows us to capture real-world behaviors in all their messy, complex glory. Unlike lab experiments where conditions are tightly controlled, observational studies let us see how people actually behave in their natural environments. It’s the difference between watching a lion in a zoo and observing one on the African savanna – both have value, but the latter gives you a much more authentic picture of the animal’s true behavior.
Another big advantage is that observation minimizes researcher influence on subjects. When people know they’re part of an experiment, they often change their behavior (consciously or unconsciously). This is known as the Fishbowl Effect in psychology , where being observed alters behavior. Observational methods, especially when covert, can help sidestep this issue.
Observation also allows us to study behaviors that simply can’t be manipulated ethically in a lab setting. Want to understand how people react in a crisis? You can’t exactly set a building on fire for the sake of science. But you can observe and analyze real emergency situations as they naturally occur.
The richness and detail of data gathered through observation is another major plus. You’re not just getting numbers on a scale or ticks in a box – you’re capturing the nuances of facial expressions, the subtleties of body language, the ebb and flow of social interactions. This depth of data can lead to insights that more structured methods might miss.
Last but not least, observational methods are great for discovering unexpected phenomena. When you’re not constrained by predetermined hypotheses or experimental designs, you’re free to notice and explore surprising patterns or behaviors. Some of the most groundbreaking discoveries in psychology have come from researchers simply paying attention to the unexpected.
Limitations and Challenges of Observational Research
Now, let’s not get carried away – observation isn’t a perfect method. Like any research approach, it comes with its own set of challenges and limitations. It’s important to be aware of these so we can interpret observational studies with the appropriate grain of salt.
One of the biggest hurdles is observer bias. No matter how objective we try to be, our own experiences, expectations, and preconceptions can color what we see and how we interpret it. It’s like wearing tinted glasses – they subtly change everything you look at. Researchers need to be constantly vigilant about their own biases and use techniques like multiple observers or blind coding to mitigate this issue.
Reactivity is another potential problem, especially in overt observation. Remember the Fishbowl Effect we mentioned earlier? Even when people know they’re being observed for legitimate research purposes, they might still alter their behavior. It’s like when you suddenly become hyper-aware of how you’re walking when you notice someone watching you on the street.
Ethical considerations loom large in observational studies, particularly when it comes to privacy and consent. How do we balance the need for authentic observation with respect for people’s right to privacy? It’s a thorny issue that researchers grapple with constantly, especially in the age of ubiquitous surveillance technology.
Let’s not sugarcoat it – observational research can be incredibly time-consuming and resource-intensive. Gathering, coding, and analyzing observational data is often a laborious process. It’s not uncommon for researchers to spend hours poring over video footage or field notes to extract meaningful insights.
Another limitation is the difficulty in establishing causality. While observation is great for describing what happens, it’s not always so great at explaining why it happens. We might see a correlation between two behaviors, but without the controlled conditions of an experiment, it’s hard to say definitively that one caused the other.
Finally, there’s the question of generalizability. How much can we extrapolate from observations of a specific group or situation to broader populations or contexts? It’s a bit like trying to understand all of human nature by watching a single family – you might gain some valuable insights, but you’d be missing a lot of the bigger picture.
Applications of the Observation Method in Psychology
Despite these challenges, the observation method continues to be a vital tool across various branches of psychology. Let’s take a whirlwind tour of some of the exciting ways researchers are putting observation to work.
In developmental psychology, observation is king. From studying how infants bond with their caregivers to tracking the complex social dynamics of adolescent peer groups, observational methods provide invaluable insights into how we grow and change over time. It’s through careful observation that we’ve learned so much about crucial concepts like attachment theory and social learning.
Social psychology researchers rely heavily on observational techniques to understand group dynamics, social influence, and interpersonal behavior. Think of classic studies like Zimbardo’s Stanford Prison Experiment or Milgram’s obedience experiments – while not purely observational, these studies leaned heavily on careful observation of participants’ behavior.
In clinical psychology, observation plays a crucial role in assessment and diagnosis. Clinicians often use structured observation techniques to evaluate symptoms, track treatment progress, and understand how mental health issues manifest in real-world settings. It’s not just about what clients say in therapy – it’s about how they behave, interact, and express themselves.
Organizational psychologists use observation to study workplace behavior, team dynamics, and leadership styles. By observing how people actually behave in professional settings (as opposed to how they say they behave), researchers can gain valuable insights into what makes organizations tick.
Cross-cultural psychology is another field where observation shines. By observing behavior across different cultural contexts, researchers can tease apart which aspects of human behavior are universal and which are culturally specific. It’s like being an anthropologist of the mind, exploring the rich diversity of human experience across the globe.
The Future of Observational Research in Psychology
As we wrap up our deep dive into the world of observational research, let’s take a moment to peer into the crystal ball and imagine what the future might hold for this venerable method.
One exciting frontier is the integration of technology into observational studies. Advances in wearable tech, virtual reality, and artificial intelligence are opening up new possibilities for capturing and analyzing behavior. Imagine AI-powered systems that can automatically code facial expressions or body language, or VR environments that allow for controlled observation of behavior in simulated real-world settings.
There’s also a growing trend towards combining observational methods with other research approaches. The empirical method in psychology is all about gathering observable, measurable evidence, and observation is a key part of that. By integrating observation with experimental designs, surveys, and physiological measures, researchers can build a more comprehensive understanding of human behavior.
The rise of big data and machine learning is also likely to impact observational research. As we generate more and more data about our daily lives through social media, smartphones, and other digital technologies, researchers have access to vast troves of observational data. The challenge will be in developing ethical and effective ways to harness this data for psychological insights.
Another area ripe for development is in addressing some of the traditional limitations of observational research. New statistical techniques and research designs are being developed to help establish causality in observational studies. And as our understanding of bias and reactivity grows, we’re getting better at designing studies that minimize these effects.
Conclusion: The Enduring Value of Observation in Psychological Science
As we’ve seen, the observation method in psychology is a powerful and versatile tool for understanding human behavior. From the subtle interactions of infants and caregivers to the complex dynamics of organizational behavior, observation allows us to peer into the rich tapestry of human experience in ways that other methods simply can’t match.
Yes, it has its challenges and limitations. But in many ways, these challenges are also opportunities – pushing us to develop new techniques, technologies, and approaches to better understand the human mind and behavior.
The future of observational research in psychology looks bright indeed. As we continue to refine our methods, integrate new technologies, and combine observation with other research approaches, we’re opening up new vistas of understanding about what makes us tick.
So the next time you find yourself people-watching in a café or observing the subtle dance of social interactions at a party, remember – you’re engaging in a time-honored tradition that has helped shape our understanding of human nature. Who knows? Your casual observations might just spark the next big question in psychological science.
After all, in the grand experiment of understanding human behavior, we’re all observers. And in that sense, the observation method isn’t just a research technique – it’s a fundamental part of what makes us human. Our curiosity, our drive to understand ourselves and others, our capacity for empathy and insight – all of these are rooted in our ability to observe and make sense of the world around us.
So here’s to observation – may it continue to illuminate the fascinating, frustrating, and endlessly complex world of human behavior for generations to come.
References:
1. Creswell, J. W., & Poth, C. N. (2016). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five approaches. Sage publications.
2. Coolican, H. (2017). Research methods and statistics in psychology. Psychology Press.
3. Mehl, M. R., & Conner, T. S. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of research methods for studying daily life. Guilford Press.
4. Robson, C., & McCartan, K. (2016). Real world research. John Wiley & Sons.
5. Bakeman, R., & Quera, V. (2011). Sequential analysis and observational methods for the behavioral sciences. Cambridge University Press.
6. Angrosino, M. (2007). Doing ethnographic and observational research. Sage.
7. McKechnie, L. E. (2008). Observational research. The SAGE encyclopedia of qualitative research methods, 2, 573-575.
8. Pellegrini, A. D. (2004). Observing children in their natural worlds: A methodological primer. Psychology Press.
9. Salkind, N. J. (2010). Encyclopedia of research design (Vol. 1). Sage.
10. Willig, C., & Rogers, W. S. (Eds.). (2017). The SAGE handbook of qualitative research in psychology. Sage.
Was this article helpful?
Would you like to add any comments (optional), leave a reply cancel reply.
Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *
Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.
Post Comment
IMAGES
VIDEO